The Interpersonal Origins of Language

social and linguistic implications of an archaeological approach to language evolution

Authors

  • Ben Marwick The Australian National University

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1558/lhs.2005.1.2.197

Keywords:

Language Evolution, Exchange Networks, Interpersonal Communication

Abstract

The development of the interpersonal functions of language is a key step in language ontogeny. Archaeological evidence of hominids moving raw materials across the landscape suggest that changes in the interpersonal communication abilities of hominids represent major events in human language evolution. The earliest hominids moved raw materials short distances, suggesting home-range sizes, social complexity and interpersonal abilities similar to those of primates. A transition from primate communication to a protolanguage is indicated by a large increase in raw-material transfer distances at about 1.2 million years ago. The increase in transfer distances results from the ability to pool social and environmental information using a protolanguage. The transition to human language is suggested by the emergence of long-distance exchange networks during the African Middle Stone Age. The operation of exchange networks requires the full panoply of human interpersonal communication abilities, such as the use of symbols in social contexts, expression of displacement, the expression of multiple degrees of intentionality and recursiveness. The results of computer simulations show that this transition from protolanguage to full language may have resulted from language adapting itself rather than any specific biological or cultural mutation.

Author Biography

  • Ben Marwick, The Australian National University

    Department of Archaeology and Natural History, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia

References

Arribas, A. and Palmqvist, P. (1999) On the ecological connection between sabre-tooths and hominids: faunal dispersal events in the lower pleistocene and a review of the evidence for the first human arrival in Europe. Journal of Archaeological Science 26: 571–85.

Baldwin, P. J., McGrew, W. C. and Tutin, C. E. (1982) Wide ranging chimpanzees at Mt Assirik, Senegal. International Journal of Primatology 3: 367–85.

Benson, J., Fries, P., Greaves, W., Iwamoto, K., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. and Taglialatela, J. (2002) Confrontation and support in bonobo-human discourse. Functions of Language 9(1): 1–38.

Bickerton, D. (1998) Catastrophic evolution: the case for a single step between protolanguage to full human language. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy and C. Knight (eds) Approaches to the Evolution of Language: social and cognitive bases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bickerton, D. (1981) Roots of Language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.

Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1984) Mental map in wild chimpanzees: an analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates 25: 160–70.

Boesch C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000) The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: behavioral ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bolus, M. and Conard, N. J. (2001) The Late Middle Palaeolithic and Earliest Upper Palaeolithic in Central Europe and their relevance for the out of Africa hypothesis. Quaternary International 75: 29–40.

Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1999) The Origins of Complex Language: an inquiry into the evolutionary beginnings of sentences, syllables and truth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Chase, P. G. and Dibble, H. L. (1987) Middle Palaeolithic symbolism: a review of current evidence and interpretations. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 6: 263–96.

Chase P. G. and Dibble, H. L. (1992) Scientific archaeology and the origins of symbolism: a reply to Bednarik. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 2(1): 43–51.

Christiansen, M. H. (1994) Infinite languages, finite minds: connectionism, learning and linguistic structures. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Edinburgh.

Davidson, I. (2002) The ‘finished artefact fallacy’: Acheulean handaxes and language origins. In A. Wray (ed.) The Transition to Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Deacon, T. W. (1997) The Symbolic Species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. Norton.

Dean, C., Leakey, M. G., Reid, D., Schrenk, F., Schwartz, G. T., Stringer, C. and Walker, A. (2001) Growth processes in teeth distinguish modern humans from Homo erectus and earlier hominins. Nature 414: 628–31.

deMenocal, P. B. (1995) Plio-Pleistocene African climate. Science 270: 53–9.

deMenocal, P. B. (2004) African climate change and faunal evolution during the PliocenePleistocene. Earth and Planetary Science Letters 220: 3–24.

d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A-M., Soressi, M., Bresson, F., Maureille, B., Nowell, A., Lakarra, J., Backwell, L. and Julien, M. (2003) Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism and music – an alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory 17(1): 1–70.

d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaerend, M. and Niekerk, K. (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 48: 3–24.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992) Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution 20: 469–93.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993) Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 681–735.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996) Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003) The social brain: mind, language and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 32: 163–81.

Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A. P. and Paabo, S. (2002) Molecular evolution of foxp2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418: 869–72.

Feblot-Augustins, J. (1997) La Circulation des Matières Premières au Paléolithique. (Etudes et Recherches Archaeologiques de l’Universite de Liège 75.) Liège: University de Liège.

Feblot-Augustins, J. and Perlès, C. (1992) Perspectives Ethnoarchéologiques sur les Échanges à Longue Distance. Ethnoarchéologie: justification, problémes, limites, XIIe recontres internationales d’archéologie et d’histoire d’Antibes 195–209. Juan-les-Pins: APDCA.

Fitch, W. T. (1999) Acoustic exaggeration of size in birds by tracheal elongation: comparative and theoretical analyses. Journal of Zoology (London) 248: 31–49.

Fitch, W. T. and Reby, D. (2001) The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 268: 1669–75.

Garber, P. A. (2000) Evidence for the use of spatial, temporal and social information by primate foragers. In S. Boinski and P. A. Garber (eds) On the Move: how and why animals travel in groups. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Gargett, R. H. (1989) Grave shortcomings: the evidence for neanderthal burial. Current Anthropology 30: 157–90.

Gargett, R. H. (1999) Middle Palaeolithic burial is not a dead issue: the view from Qafzeh, Saint-Césaire, Kebara, Amud and Dederiyeh. Journal of Human Evolution 37: 27–90.

Goldstein, D. B., Linares, A. R., Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. (1995) Genetic absolute dating based on microsatellites and the origin of modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 92: 6723–7.

Goodall, J. (1986) The Chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behaviour. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Halliday, M. A. K. (1975) Learning How to Mean: explorations in the development of language. London: Arnold.

Halliday, M. A. K. (1993) Towards a language based theory of learning. Linguistics and Education 5: 93–116.

Halliday, M. A. K. (1994) An Introduction to Functional Grammar. Second edition. London: Edward Arnold.

Halliday, M. A. K. (2004) On grammar as the driving force from primary to higher-order consciousness. In G. Williams and A. Lukin (eds) The Development of Language: functional perspectives on species and individuals. London: Continuum.

Hammer, M. F., Spurdle, A. B., Karafet, T., Bonner, M. R., Wood, E. T., Novelletto, A., Malaspina, P., Mitchell, R. J., Horai, S., Jenkins, T. and Zegura, S. L. (1997) The geographic distribution of human Y chromosome variation. Genetics 145: 787–805.

Harding, R. M., Fullerton, S. M., Griffiths, R. C., Bond, J., Cox, M. J., Schneider, J. A., Moulin, D. S. and Clegg, J. B. (1997) Archaic African and Asian lineages in the genetic ancestry of modern humans. American Journal of Human Genetics 60: 772–89.

Harpending, H. C., Sherry, S. T., Rogers, A. R. and Stoneking, M. (1993) The genetic structure of ancient human populations. Current Anthropology 34: 483–96.

Harpending, H. C., Batzer, M. A., Gurven, M., Jorde, L. B., Rogers, A. R. and Sherry, S. T. (1998) Genetic traces of ancient demography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 95: 1961–7.

Harris, E. and Hey, J. (1999) X Chromosome evidence for ancient human histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 96: 3320–24.

Hayden, B. (1993) The cultural capacities of Neanderthals: a review and re-evaluation. Journal of Human Evolution 24: 113–46.

Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Marean, C., Milo, R. and Yates, R. (2001) An early bone tool industry from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa: implications for the origins of modern human behaviour, symbolism and language. Journal of Human Evolution 41: 631–78.

Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, G. A. T., Mercier, N., Sealy, J. C., Valladas, H., Watts, I. and Wintle, A. G. (2002) Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295: 127–1280.

Henshilwood, C. S. and Marean, C. W. (2003) The origin of modern human behaviour: critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology 44(5): 627–51.

Henshilwood, C. S. and Sealy, J. C. (1997) Bone artefacts from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, Southern Cape, South Africa. Current Anthropology 38: 890–95.

Hewlett, B. S., von de Koppel, J. and Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986) Exploration and mating range of Aka Pygmies of the Central African Republic. In L. L. Cavalli-Sforza (ed.) African Pygmies. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Holloway, R. L. (1983) Human Paleonotological evidence relevant to language behavior. Human Neurobiology 2: 105–14.

Hublin, J. J., Spoor, F., Braun, M., Zonneveld, F. and Condemi, S. (1996) A Late Neanderthal associated with Upper Paleolithic artifacts. Nature 381: 224–6.

Hurford, J. (1989) Biological evolution of the Saussurean sign as a component of the language acquisition device. Lingua 77: 187–222.

Jackendoff, R. (2002) Foundations of Language: brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Joffe, T. H. (1997) Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. Journal of Human Evolution 32: 593–605.

Kaminski, J., Call, J. and Fischer, J. (2004) Word learning in a domestic dog: evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science 304: 1682–3.

Karavanic, I. and Smith, F. (1998) The Middle/Upper Paleolithic interface and the relationship of Neanderthals and early modern humans in the Hrvatsko Zagorje, Croatia. Journal of Human Evolution 34: 223–48.

Keates, S. G. (2004) Home range size in Middle Pleistocene China and human dispersal patterns in Eastern and Central Asia. Asian Perspectives 43: 227–47.

Kelly, R. L. (1983) Hunter-gatherer mobility strategies. Journal of Anthropological Research 39: 277–306.

Kelly, R. L. (1995) The Foraging Spectrum: diversity in hunter-gatherer lifeways. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.

Kirby, S. (2000) Syntax without natural selection: how compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In C. Knight, M. Stoddert-Kennedy and J. R. Hurford (eds) The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kirby, S. and Hurford, J. (1997) Learning, culture and evolution in the origin of linguistic constraints. In P. Husbands and S. Rasmussen (eds) Artificial Life II. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Kirby, S. and Hurford, J. (2001) The emergence of linguistic structure: an overview of the iterated learning model. In A. Cangelosi and D. Parisi (eds) Simulating the Evolution of Language. London: Springer Verlag.

Klein, R. G. (1999) The Human Career. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Lahr, M. M. and Foley, R. A. (1998) Towards a theory of modern human origins: geography, demography and diversity on recent human evolution. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 41: 137–76.

Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. and Monaco, A. P. (2001) A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 519–23.

Lévêque, F., Bocker, A. M. and Guilbaud, M. (1993) Context of a late Neanderthal: implications of multidisciplinary research for the transition to Upper Palaeolithic adaptations at Sainte-Cesaire, Charente-Maritime, France. Madison: Prehistory Press.

Lieberman, P. (1992) On the evolution of human language. In J. A. Hawkins and M. GellMann (eds) The Evolution of Human Language. New York: Addison-Wesley.

Lieberman, P. (1994) Biologically bound behavior, free-will and human evolution. In J. I. Casti and A. Karlqvist (ed.) Conflict and Cooperation in General Evolutionary Processes. New York: Wiley.

Marcus, G. F. and Fisher, S. E. (2003) foxp2 in focus: what can genes tell us about speech and language? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(6): 256–62.

Marwick, B. (2003) Pleistocene exchange networks as evidence for the evolution of language. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 13(1): 67–81.

McBrearty, S. and Brooks, A. S. (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behaviour. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453–563.

McGrew, W. C., Baldwin, P. J. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1981) Chimpanzees in a hot, dry and open habitat: Mt Assirik, Senegal, West Africa. Journal of Human Evolution 10: 227–44.

Mellars, P. A. (1996) Symbolism, language and the Neanderthal mind. In P. A. Mellars and K. R. Gibson (eds) Modelling the Early Human Mind. (McDonald Institute Monograph.) Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

Mellars, P. A. and Stringer, C. (1989) The Human Revolution: behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Miller, G. H., Beaumont, P. B., Brooks, A. S., Deacon, H. J., Hare, P. E. and Jull, A. J. T. (1999) Earliest modern humans in South Africa dated by isoleucine epimerization in ostrich eggshell. Quaternary Science Review 18: 1537–48.

Mithen, S. (1996) The Prehistory of the Mind: the cognitive origins of art, religion and science. London: Thames and Hudson.

Munroe, S. and Cangelosi, A. (2002) Learning and the evolution of language: the role of cultural variation and learning cost in the Baldwin effect. Artificial Life 8(4): 311–39.

Nei, M. and Roychoudhury, A. K. (1974) Genetic variation within and between the three major races of man: Caucasoids, Negroids and Mongoloids. American Journal of Human Genetics 26: 421–43.

Newmeyer, F. (1991) Functional explanation in linguistics and the origin of language. Language and Communication 11: 1–28.

Noble, W. and Davidson, I. (1996) Human Evolution, Language and Mind: a psychological and archaeological inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Nowak, M. A. and Komarova, N. (2001) Towards an evolutionary theory of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5(7): 288–95.

Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. and Niyogi, P. (2001) Evolution of universal grammar. Science 291: 114–18.

Nowak, M. A. and Krakauer, D. C. (1999) The evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 96: 8028–33.

Nowak, M. A., Krakauer, D. C. and Dress, A. (1999) An error limit for the evolution of language. Proceedings of The Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 2131–6.

Ovchinnikov, I. V., Götherströms, A., Romanova, P. R., Kharitonov, V. M., Lidens, K. and Goodwin, W. (2000) Molecular analysis of Neanderthal DNA from the Northern Caucasus. Nature 404: 490–93.

Painter, C. (2004) The ‘interpersonal first’ principle in child language development. In G. Williams and A. Lukin (eds) The Development of Language: functional perspectives on species and individuals. New York, NY: Continuum.

Paquette, D. (1992) Object exchange between captive chimpanzees: a case report. Human Evolution 7: 11–15.

Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–86.

Reed, K. E. (1997) Early hominid evolution and ecological change through the African Plio-Pleistocene. Journal of Human Evolution 32: 289–322.

Reich, D. E. and Goldstein, D. B. (1998) Genetic evidence for a Palaeolithic human population expansion in Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 95: 8119–23.

Reynolds, R. G. and Zeigler, B. P. (1979) A formal mathematical model for the operation of consensus based hunting-gathering bands. In C. Renfrew and K. L. Cook (eds) Transformations: mathematical approaches to culture change. New York: Academic Press.

Roebroeks, W. (2001) Hominid behaviour and the earliest occupation of Europe: an exploration. Journal of Human Evolution 41: 437–61.

Roebroeks, W., Kolen, J. and Rensik, E. (1988) Planning depth, anticipation and the organization of Middle Palaeolithic technology: the ‘archaic natives’ meet Eve’s descendants. Helenium 28: 17–34.

Semaw, S. (2000) The world’s oldest stone artefacts from Gona, Ethiopia: their implications for understanding stone technology and patterns of human evolution between 2·6–1·5 million years ago. Journal of Archaeological Science 27: 1197–214.

Smith, B. H. and Tompkins, R. L. (1995) Toward a life history of the hominidae. Annual Review of Anthropology 24: 257–79.

Steele, J. (1996) On predicting hominid group sizes. In J. Steele and S. Shennan The Archaeology of Human Ancestry: power, sex and tradition. London: Routledge.

Stone, V. E., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Kroll, N. and Knight, R. T. (2002) Selective impairment of reasoning about social exchange in a patient with bilateral limbic system damage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 99(17): 11531–6.

Stoneking M., Fontius, J. J., Clifford, S. L., Soodyall, H., Arcot, S. S., Saha, N., Jenkins, T., Tahir, M. A., Deininger, P. L. and Batzer, M. A. (1997) Alu insertion polymorphisms and human evolution: evidence for a larger population size in Africa. Genome Research 7: 1061–71.

Sugiyamaa, L. S., Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (2002) Cross-cultural evidence of cognitive adaptations for social exchange among the Shiwiar of Ecuadorian Amazonia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 99(17): 11537–42.

Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Rumbaugh, D. M., Benson, J. and Greaves, W. (2004) Language, apes and meaning-making. In G. Williams and A. Lukin (eds) The Development of Language: functional perspectives on species and individuals. New York: Continuum.

Thieme, H. (1997) Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature 385: 807–10.

Tonkes, B. and J. Wiles (2002) Methodological issues in stimulating the emergence of language. In A. Wray (ed.) The Transition to Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Turner, A. (1990) The evolution of the guild of larger terrestrial carnivores in the PlioPleistocene of Africa. Geobios 23: 349–68.

Turner, A. (1992) Large carnivores and earliest European hominids: changing determinants of resource availability during the Lower and Middle Pleistocene. Journal of Human Evolution 22: 109–26.

Villa, P. and d’Errico, F. (2001) Bone and ivory points in the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic of Europe. Journal of Human Evolution 41: 69–112.

Vrba, E. S. (1995) The fossil record of African antelopes (Mammalia, Bovidae) in relation to human evolution and paleoclimate. In E. S. Vrba, H. H. Denton, T. C. Partridge and L. H. Burckle (eds) Paleoclimate and Evolution with Emphasis on Human Origins. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Watts, D. P. and Mitani, J. C. (2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138: 299–327.

Whallon, R. (1989) Elements of culture change in the Later Palaeolithic. In P. A. Mellars and C. Stringer (eds) The Human Revolution: behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Wilson, M. L. and Wrangham, R. W. (2003) Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology 32: 363–92.

Wobst, H. M. (1976) Locational relationships in Palaeolithic Society. Journal of Human Evolution 5: 49–58.

Wrangham, R. W. (1987) The significance of African apes for reconstructing human social evolution. In W. G. Kinzey (ed.) The Evolution of Human Behaviour: primate models. Albany: State University of New York Press.

Wray, A. (2000) Holistic utterances in protolanguage: the link from primates to humans. In C. Knight, M. Stoddert-Kennedy and J. R. Hurford (eds) The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Published

2007-02-18

Issue

Section

Articles

How to Cite

Marwick, B. (2007). The Interpersonal Origins of Language: social and linguistic implications of an archaeological approach to language evolution. Linguistics and the Human Sciences, 1(2), 197-224. https://doi.org/10.1558/lhs.2005.1.2.197

Most read articles by the same author(s)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 > >>